Leftward Spatial Bias in Dialyzed Patients with End-Stage Renal Disease: A Sign of Right Hemispheric Attentional Activation or Impaired Left-Sided Disengagement?

Keywords: dialysis, line bisection, attentional bias, attentional disengagement, global attention, focal attention

Abstract

In the horizontal line bisection task, patients with end-stage renal disease (ESRD) treated with dialysis typically have an increase in the normal leftward attentional bias. It is not clear, however, whether this increased bias is related to an increase of attention toward left hemispace or impaired disengagement from left-sided stimuli. Non-demented patients with ESRD who were being treated with dialysis and demographically matched healthy controls attempted to bisect series of horizontal lines composed of two segments of unequal length and width, with the longer segment placed to the right or left. To test for the presence of a global spatial attentional bias, participants attempted to find the middle of the whole line. To test for focal attentional engagement and attentional disengagement, participants were asked to bisect only the longer segment of these compound lines. For the global condition, both patients and healthy controls were biased toward the longer segment and more so to the left. In the focal condition, when the longer segment was on the right side, both groups deviated their bisections toward the shorter segment. However, when in the focal condition the longer segment was on the left healthy controls deviated their bisection significantly more rightwards than did the patients, whereas with the longer segment on the right, the patients and healthy controls did not differ in their bias. These results suggest that the previously recognized dysfunction of the right fronto-subcortical attentional network in dialyzed patients might account for the results of the current study.

References

Barnett, N. P., Apodaca, T. R., Magill, M., Colby, S. M., Gwaltney, C., Rohsenow, D. J., & Monti, P. M. (2010). Moderators and mediators of two brief interventions for alcohol in the emergency department. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 105(3), 452–465. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02814.x

Bonato, M., Romeo, Z., Blini, E., Pitteri, M., Durgoni, E., Passarini, L., Meneghello, F., Zorzi, M., & Anderson, B. (2019). Ipsilesional impairments of visual awareness after right-hemispheric stroke. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00697

Bowen, A., McKenna, K., & Tallis, R. C. (1999). Reasons for variability in the reported rate of occurrence of unilateral spatial neglect after stroke. Stroke, 30(6), 1196–1202. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.30.6.1196

Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18(4–5), 491–498.

Bultitude, J. H., & Aimola Davies, A. M. (2006). Putting attention on the line: Investigating the activation—orientation hypothesis of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1849–1858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.001

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100.

Corbetta, M., Kincade, M. J., Lewis, C., Snyder, A. Z., & Sapir, A. (2005). Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1574

Denny-Brown, D., & Chambers, R. A. (1958). The parietal lobe and behavior. Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 36, 35–117.

Dixit, A., Dhawan, S., Raizada, A., Yadav, A., Vaney, N., & Kalra, O. P. (2013). Attention and information processing in end stage renal disease and effect of hemodialysis: A bedside study. Renal Failure, 35(9), 1246–1250. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/0886022X.2013.819768

Dolcos, F., Rice, H. J., & Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: Right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 7634(July 2016), 819–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00068-4

Falchook, A. D., Salazar, L., Neal, D., Kesayan, T., Williamson, J. B., Malaty, I. A., McFarland, N. R., Okun, M. S., Rodriguez, R. L., Wagle Shukla, A., & Heilman, K. M. (2015). Global attentional neglect of segmented lines in Parkinson’s disease. Neurocase, 21(4), 501–508. https://doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2014.942671

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886

Harciarek, M., Michałowski, J., Biedunkiewicz, B., Williamson, J., Dębska-Ślizień, A., Rutkowski, B., & Heilman, K. M. (2016). Disorders of the anterior attentional-intentional system in patients with end stage renal disease: Evidence from reaction time studies. Brain and Cognition, 107, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2016.05.005

Harciarek, M., Williamson, J. B., Burtis, B., Haque, S., & Heilman, K. M. (2012). Ipsilateral neglect from a subcortical lesion. Cognitive And Behavioral Neurology, 25(1), 42–49. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0b013e3182491465

Heilman, K. M., & Valenstein, E. (1979). Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect. Annals of Neurology, 5(2), 166–170. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.410050210

Heilman, K. M., & Valenstein, E. (2003). Clinical-neuropsychology, Fourth Edition. European Journal of Neurology, 10(5), 606. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1468-1331.2003.00655.x

Heilman, K. M., & Valenstein, E. (2012). Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.

Heilman, K. M., & Van Den Abell, T. (1980). Right hemisphere dominance for attention: The mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology, 30(3), 327–330.

Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: A review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 93–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7

Kim, M., Na, D. L., Kim, G. M., Adair, J. C., Lee, K. H., & Heilman, K. M. (1999). Ipsilesional neglect: Behavioural and anatomical features. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 67(1), 35–38. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.67.1.35

Kinsbourne, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, 33, 193–201.

Kurella Tamura, M., Vittinghoff, E., Yang, J., Go, A. S., Seliger, S. L., Kusek, J. W., Lash, J., Cohen, D. L., Simon, J., Batuman, V., Ordonez, J., Makos, G., & Yaffe, K. (2016). Anemia and risk for cognitive decline in chronic kidney disease. BMC Nephrology, 17(13), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12882-016-0226-6

Kwon, S. E., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). Ipsilateral neglect in a patient following a unilateral frontal lesion. Neurology, 41(12), 2001–2004. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.41.12.2001

Lynch, J. C. (1980). The functional organization of posterior parietal association cortex. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(4), 485–499.

Majkowicz, M. (1994). Skala HAD [The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale—HAD]. In K. de Walden-Galuszko & M. Majkowicz (Eds.), Jakość życia w chorobie nowotworowej [Quality of life in malignant disease] (pp. 153–158). Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Gdańskiego.

Mańkowska, A., Heilman, K. M., Williamson, J. B., Biedunkiewicz, B., Dębska-Ślizień, A., & Harciarek, M. (2017). Leftward bias of visual attention in patients with end-stage renal disease receiving dialysis: A neglected phenomenon. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 30(4), 176–181.

Mańkowska, A., Heilman, K. M., Williamson, J. B., & Harciarek, M. (2019). Age-related changes in the allocation of spatially directed focal attention. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 24(10), 1121–1124. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617718000620

Michałowski, J. M., Harciarek, M., Biedunkiewicz, B., Williamson, J., Debska-Ślizień, A., Rutkowski, B., & Heilman, K. M. (2016). Slowing with end-stage renal disease: Attentive but unprepared to act. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 106, 30–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.06.002

Mosquera, D. M., Lamb, D. G., Garcia, A. M., Rodriguez, J. A., Correa, L. N., Salazar, L., Schwartz, Z. J., Cohen, R. A., Falchook, A. D., & Heilman, K. M. (2018). Asymmetrical distractibility of global and focal visuospatial attention during segmental and total compound line bisections. Neuropsychology, 32(7), 843–849. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000455

Murray, A. M. (2009). Editorial: The brain and the kidney connection: A model of accelerated vascular cognitive impairment. Neurology, 73(12), 916–917. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b99a2e

Pardo, J. V., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1991). Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature, 349(6304), 61–64. https://doi.org/10.1038/349061a0

Pereira, A. A., Weiner, D. E., Scott, T., & Sarnak, M. J. (2005). Cognitive function in dialysis patients. American Journal of Kidney Diseases, 45(3), 448–462. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.ajkd.2004.10.024

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231

Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

Reuter-Lorenz, P., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 266(190), 240–266. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/027826269090018J

Robertson, L. C., Lamb, M. R., & Knight, R. T. (1988). Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 8(10), 3757–3769. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.54702

Sacchetti, D. L., Goedert, K. M., Foundas, A. L., & Barrett, A. M. (2015). Ipsilesional neglect: Behavioral and anatomical correlates. Neuropsychology, 29(2), 183–190. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000122

Shuren, J. E., Jacobs, D. H., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). The influence of center of mass effect on the distribution of spatial attention in the vertical and horizontal dimensions. Brain and Cognition, 34(2), 293–300. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1997.0918

Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Shallice, T., Picton, T. W., Binns, M. A., Macdonald, R., Borowiec, A., & Katz, D. I. (2005). Multiple frontal systems controlling response speed. Neuropsychologia, 43(3), 396–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.010

Wang, C., Burtis, D. B., Ding, M., Mo, J., Williamson, J. B., & Heilman, K. M. (2018). The effects of left and right monocular viewing on hemispheric activation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40(2), 198–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2017.1332169

Williamson, J. B., Lamb, D. G., Burtis, D. B., Haque, S., M. Zilli, E., Kesayan, T., Harciarek, M., & Heilman, K. M. (2018). Right hemispatial ipsilesional neglect with chronic right hemisphere strokes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40(4), 347–356. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2017.1347606

Wright, D., Makin, A. D. J., & Bertamini, M. (2015). Right-lateralized alpha desynchronization during regularity discrimination: Hemispheric specialization or directed spatial attention? Psychophysiology, 52(5), 638–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12399

Published
2022-02-02
Section
Articles